When and why ornaments advertise an animal’s number of parasites

Post by Faith Rovenolt

A recent study pub­lished in Evol­u­tion Let­ters invest­ig­ates the impact of eco­lo­gic­al selec­tion pres­sures on thermal plastic responses in growth sched­ules in insect. Coau­thor Faith Roven­olt tells us more: 

Reindeer have their antlers, pea­cocks their feath­ers, and Trin­id­a­di­an gup­pies their colored spots (see image). All of these, and many oth­er beau­ti­ful and bizarre traits in anim­als, can help an indi­vidu­al stand out from a crowd and impress a mate. Like orna­ments on a Christ­mas tree, they can make an anim­al sparkle and shine—but are they mere baubles or more like bill­boards, advert­ising how great a mate their own­er would make? In oth­er words, what do sexu­al orna­ments sig­nal to poten­tial mates?

A photograph showing three individual Trinidadian guppies with various patterns. One is solid silver, one has small orange and gray spots, and another has striking orange and black spots.
A female Trin­id­a­di­an guppy (bot­tom left) with two males (top left and right). These males dif­fer in the qual­ity of their sexu­al orna­ments. Each of these orna­ments also dif­fers in their dynam­ism: the size of a male’s orange spots is fixed at matur­ity but the saturation—or the col­or intensity—of those same spots can change with­in days. Mean­while, a male’s court­ing behavior—which involves a male twist­ing his body like in the top left—can change with­in seconds. The first of these orna­ments is clas­si­fied as stat­ic in our study while the lat­ter two are dynam­ic. Image Cred­it: Faith Rovenolt

One pos­sib­il­ity is that anim­als with the largest, bright­est, or shi­ni­est orna­ments have the few­est para­sites. Thus, if the anim­als with the best orna­ments had an advert­ising slo­gan, it might be: “Look at me! I’m so sexy, I’m para­site-free”. A couple of meta-ana­lyses (sci­entif­ic stud­ies that com­bine data from mul­tiple oth­er stud­ies) pub­lished in the 2000’s and earli­er found just that—a neg­at­ive cor­rel­a­tion between the num­ber of para­sites a host had and the qual­ity of their orna­ments. How­ever, those stud­ies left unanswered ques­tions: what are the bene­fits of mat­ing with highly orna­men­ted mates and what kind of orna­ments are best at advert­ising hav­ing few­er parasites?

My co-authors and I decided to answer these ques­tions by con­duct­ing a meta-ana­lys­is ourselves, tak­ing advant­age of the hun­dreds of addi­tion­al stud­ies con­duc­ted on this ques­tion since the pre­vi­ous meta-ana­lyses. We combed through these stud­ies, gath­er­ing the data from each, and then put it all togeth­er to see if we could find a neg­at­ive cor­rel­a­tion between a host’s num­ber of para­sites and their orna­ment qual­ity. We also grabbed inform­a­tion about the host and para­site, includ­ing wheth­er the para­site infec­ted the inside of the host (like an intest­in­al worm) or the out­side (like a tick). By record­ing these factors, we could then test if any of them affected our results—for example, is there a stronger rela­tion­ship between orna­ment qual­ity and a host’s num­ber of para­sites if the para­sites are found on the out­side instead of the inside? We ended up with more than 400 data points, much more than the pre­vi­ous meta-analyses.

Like the pre­vi­ous meta-ana­lyses, we found that—on average—there was a weak but neg­at­ive cor­rel­a­tion between the num­ber of para­sites hosts had and their orna­ment qual­ity. That means, in gen­er­al, a mate with bet­ter, bright­er, or fan­ci­er sexu­al orna­ments was likely to have few­er para­sites than one with worse, duller, or plain­er ones. Import­antly, our study also found that only two of the addi­tion­al factors we col­lec­ted affected our res­ults: wheth­er a para­site could trans­mit dur­ing sex or if the orna­ment could dynam­ic­ally change.

We found that only those para­sites that could trans­mit between hosts dur­ing sex had a neg­at­ive link with orna­ment qual­ity. Para­sites can trans­mit dur­ing sex not only if they’re a sexu­ally trans­mit­ted infec­tion but also if they’re trans­mit­ted by close con­tact in gen­er­al. By choos­ing mates with few­er para­sites, then, anim­als might have benefited by avoid­ing get­ting infec­ted. If para­sites can’t trans­mit dur­ing sex—like if, for example, a para­site needs to infect a dif­fer­ent host spe­cies before it again poses a risk to the first species—then choos­ing a para­site-free mate doesn’t carry that same bene­fit and thus might explain why their hosts’ orna­ments didn’t advert­ise the num­ber of parasites.

We also found that orna­ment dynamism—or how fast an orna­ment could change in quality—mattered. If an orna­ment couldn’t change qual­ity dur­ing a breed­ing season—like the size of a reindeer’s antlers—we called it stat­ic. Mean­while, if it could change qual­ity dur­ing a breed­ing season—like the shini­ness of a bird’s feathers—we con­sidered it dynam­ic. Import­antly, we con­sidered beha­vi­or to be a kind of dynam­ic ornament—how often a roost­er courts a hen can change must faster than the size of his comb, but more fre­quent dis­plays are a high­er qual­ity “orna­ment” in the same way as lar­ger comb. We found that only dynam­ic orna­ments were neg­at­ively cor­rel­ated with the num­ber of para­sites an anim­al had. This makes sense, as stat­ic orna­ments are grown in the past, and so may not be a good indic­at­or of the cur­rent num­ber of para­sites. Thus, we found more dynam­ic orna­ments were the best kind of traits to advert­ise hav­ing few­er para­sites. This held true even when para­sites couldn’t trans­mit dur­ing sex, sug­gest­ing that anim­als choos­ing a more orna­men­ted mate under those con­di­tions could have benefited by hav­ing off­spring that inher­it the orna­men­ted mate’s abil­ity to fight off parasites. 

Alto­geth­er, our work sug­gests that para­sites do play an import­ant role in how anim­als chose their mates, and that orna­ment qual­ity con­veys inform­a­tion about para­site infec­tion if orna­ments are dynam­ic or para­sites can trans­mit dur­ing sex. We hope our study inspires fur­ther research into how anim­als choos­ing their mates affects para­sites and vice versa. 

The Steph­en­son Lab is loc­ated in the Depart­ment of Bio­lo­gic­al Sci­ences at the Uni­ver­sity of Pitt­s­burgh. The ori­gin­al art­icle is freely avail­able to read and down­load from Evol­u­tion Letters.

Leave a Reply